Skip to main content
Log in

The diachronic syntax of negated adjuncts in English

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper we investigate the diachronic changes in negation and Emphatic Focus that are responsible for the distribution of negated adjuncts in Present Day English. These can occur clause-medially and clause-initially, but generally not clause-finally. While clause-initial negated adjuncts move to the left-periphery triggering Negative Inversion for emphasis, clause-medial negated adjuncts are argued to occur in their first-merged position as vP-adjuncts. We relate the inability of clause-final negated adjuncts to express sentential negation to the loss of Prosodic-movement and Negative Concord in the transition from Late Middle English to Early Modern English. The eventual loss of Negative Concord is related to the reanalysis of negative words from non-negative (i.e. [uNeg]) to negative (i.e. [iNeg]). Upon loss of Prosodic-movement, reanalysis of negative words as [iNeg] results in the rise of Negative Inversion to express Focus.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Accessible at http://quod.lib.umich.edu/m/mec/.

  2. Throughout the paper, we adopt Zeijlstra’s (2012) notion of Agree, also known as Reverse Agree. Unlike Chomsky’s (2000, 2001) Agree, where Probes scan their c-command domain for a Goal that can value (and thus check) an uninterpretable unvalued formal feature that the Probe carries, Zeijlstra assumes Agree to be upwards, i.e. with a Goal c-commanding the Probe. In the specific case of NC, this amounts to a Goal with an [iNeg] feature c-commanding a Probe with a [uNeg] feature, as shown in (10b).

  3. Whereas other kinds of movement (e.g. wh-movement, DP-raising, etc.) are feature-driven, Prosodic-movement takes place for prosodic reasons. Other types of movement, e.g. Müller’s (2004) Edge Domain Pied Piping evacuation vP-movement (which has been argued to result in V2 structures in German), are also claimed not to be motivated by formal features. See also Richards (2016) for a critical discussion on the role of formal features as triggers of movement, and on the interaction of prosody and syntax.

  4. Unlike English, Spanish does not have the option of placing nuclear stress in clause internal-positions to coincide with informational focus (Zubizarreta 1998).

  5. Word-forms in post-medieval examples are taken not to require glossing to be comprehensible.

  6. Such correlation is also discussed in Wallage (2012). Both Ingham (2007) and Wallage (2012) observe that in Middle English subject-verb inversion following clause-initial negated arguments and adverbials takes place regardless of whether there is NC or not. When NC involves not in ME and EMnE, no inversion is attested with clause-initial negative elements, but when NC involves ne, NI is attested with a clause-initial negative element.

  7. Verse texts were excluded because the exigencies of versification could have affected the use of NC.

  8. When an exceptional phenomenon is so rare in historical data, where grammaticality intuitions are not available, accidental error must be considered a likely explanation. We henceforth assume that the co-occurrence with not found elsewhere indicates grammatical regularity.

  9. The conflict between the result of Prosodic-movement and the requirement of sentential negation to have scope over the vP can be envisaged as resulting from one of these constraints being ranked higher than the other (as in Optimality Theory): if assumed that the scope requirement of sentential negation is ranked higher than prosodic/informational alignment, Prosodic-movement will only apply if its output does not violate the higher-ranked constraint.

  10. The Old Bailey Trials corpus is available at https://www.oldbaileyonline.org/. Reference numbers given will direct the reader to the texts. Accessed 27 March 2019.

  11. Similar data are discussed in Tubau (2016) for Traditional Dialects of British English.

  12. An anonymous reviewer points out that our analysis makes the testable prediction that those varieties of English that allow NC as part of their grammar should mirror ME in permitting clause-final negated adjuncts. While the 18th century data confirm such prediction, so do contemporary data, (i).

    1. (i)

      The shit ain’t funny in no way.

    (Retrieved from http://www.bluelight.org/vb/archive/index.php/t-496656.html. Originally posted in 2010).

    Whether examples such as (i) are derived by means of Prosodic-movement or else contain the negated adjunct in a VP-adjoined position is an open question that must be left for further research. To answer such a question it would be necessary to investigate whether Prosodic-movement is observed in other contexts in contemporary varieties of English that allow NC. No such research is known to us.

  13. Reanalysis of neg-words and the sentential negative marker from [iNeg] to ¬∃ (neg-words) and to ¬ (negative marker) is assumed to have taken place after reanalysis of [uNeg]-neg-words to [iNeg]-neg-words resulted in [iNeg]-specified lexical items not engaging in Agree chains with [uNeg]-specified lexical items. We assume that this would lead to the formal feature [iNeg] being reanalysed as semantic negation (cf. Zeijlstra 2004).

References

  • Baker, Carl Leroy. 1970. Double negatives. Linguistic Inquiry 1: 169–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biberauer, Theresa, and Ian Roberts. 2011. Negative words and related expressions: a new perspective on some familiar puzzles. In The evolution of negation: Beyond the Jespersen Cycle, eds. Richard Ingham and Pierre Larrivée, 23–60. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosque, Ignacio. 1980. Sobre la Negación. Madrid: Ediciones Cátedra.

    Google Scholar 

  • Büring, Daniel. 2004. Negative inversion. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 35, eds. Leah Bateman and Cherlon Ussery, 1–20. University of Massachusetts: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by Step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka, 89–156. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 1–54. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Clercq, Karen. 2010. Neg-shift in English. Evidence from PP-adjuncts. In 12th Seoul International Conference on Generative Grammar. 2010 Movement in Minimalism, eds. Duh-Ho An and Soo-Yeon Kim. Seoul: Hankuk Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giannakidou, Anastasia, and Josep Quer. 1997. Two mechanisms for the licensing of negative indefinites. In Formal Linguistics Society of Mid-America (FLSM) 6, eds. Leslie Gabriele, Debra Hardison, and Robert Westmoreland, 103–114. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 1995. The syntax of negation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2001. Negative preposing, negative inversion and the split CP. In Negation and polarity: Syntactic and semantic perspectives, eds. Laurence R. Horn and Yashiko Kato, 21–61. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane, and Raffaella Zanuttini. 1991. Negative heads and the NEG-criterion. The Linguistic Review 8: 233–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herburger, Elena. 2001. The Negative concord puzzle revisited. Natural Language Semantics 9: 289–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, Richard. 2005a. The loss of Neg V -> C in Middle English. Linguistische Berichte 202: 171–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, Richard. 2005b. NegV1 and secondary negation in Old and Middle English religious prose. In York Papers in Linguistics 2(3), eds. Joanne Close, Alexandra Galani, Beck Sinar and Phillip Wallage, 29–49. University of York: Department of Language and Linguistic Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, Richard. 2007. NegP and negated constituent movement. Transactions of the Philological Society 105: 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, Richard. 2013. The syntax of negation in the history of English. In Negation in the languages of Europe and the Mediterranean, eds. David Willis, Christopher Lucas, and Anne Breitbarth, 119–150. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobsson, Bengt. 1951. Inversion in English. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jack, George B. 1978a. Negation in Later Middle English Prose. Archivum Linguisticum 9: 58–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jack, George B. 1978b. Negative Concord in Early Middle English. Studia Neophilologica 50: 29–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jespersen, Otto. 1922. Language: Its nature, development, and origin. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kallel, Amel. 2007. The loss of negative concord in Standard English: Internal factors. Language Variation and Change 19: 27–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kallel, Amel. 2011. The loss of negative concord in Standard English: A case of lexical reanalysis. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klima, Edward S. 1964. Negation in English. In The structure of language, eds. Jerry A. Fodor and Jerrold J. Katz, 246–323. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, William. 1972. Negative attraction and negative concord in English grammar. Language 48: 773–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, William. 1992. Expressing negation. In Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) 2, eds. Chris Barker and David Dowty. 237–259. Columbus: Ohio State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, William. 1994. Thetic and categorical, stage and individual, weak and strong. In Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT) 4, eds. Mandy Harvey and Lynn Santelman. 220–229. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laka, Itziar. 1990. Negation in syntax: On the nature of functional categories and projections. PhD diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Mazzon, Gabriella. 2004. A history of English negation. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, Gereon. 2004. Verb-second as vP-First. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 7: 179–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nevalainen, Terttu. 1997. Recycling inversion: The case of initial adverbs and negators in Early Modern English. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 31: 203–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevalainen, Terttu, and Helena Raumolin-Brunberg. 2003. Historical sociolinguistics: Language change in Tudor and Stuart England. Oxon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penka, Doris. 2007. Negative indefinites. PhD diss., University of Tübingen.

  • Pollock, Jean-Yves. 1989. Verb movement, Universal Grammar, and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry 20(3): 365–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Norvin. 2016. Contiguity theory. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tottie, Gunnel. 1991. Negation in English speech and writing: A study in variation. London: Academic Press Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tubau, Susagna. 2016. Lexical variation and negative concord in traditional dialects of British English. The Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 19(2): 143–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tubau, Susagna, and Richard Ingham. 2015. Some historical notes on English negation: unethes, almost and hardly. In Studies in linguistic variation and change: From Old to Middle English, eds. Fabienne Toupin and Brian Lowrey, 215–226. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallage, Philip. 2012. Negative inversion, negative concord and sentential negation in the history of English. English Language and Linguistics 16(1): 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanuttini, Raffaella. 1991. Syntactic properties of sentential negation: A comparative study of Romance languages. PhD diss., University of Pennsylvania.

  • Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2004. Sentential negation and negative concord. PhD diss., University of Amsterdam.

  • Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2012. There is only one way to agree. The Linguistic Review 29(3): 491–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zubizarreta, Maria Luisa. 1998. Prosody, focus, and word order. Linguistic Inquiry Monograph 33. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research has been funded by two research grants awarded by the Spanish Ministerio de Indústria, Economía y Competitividad (FFI2017-8254-P, FFI2016-81750-REDT), and by a grant awarded by the Generalitat de Catalunya to the Centre de Lingüística Teòrica (2017SGR634). We would like to thank three anonymous reviewers and the handling editor for comments and suggestions on how to improve the manuscript. All errors remain our own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susagna Tubau.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ingham, R., Tubau, S. The diachronic syntax of negated adjuncts in English. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 38, 477–497 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-019-09450-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-019-09450-1

Keywords

Navigation