Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Weak English foreign language readers: the cross-linguistic impact of morphological awareness

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Linguistic abilities as well as home environment explain successful English as a foreign language (EFL) reading acquisition. The aim of this study was to examine the extent to which morphological derivational awareness measured in Arabic as a first language (L1) predicted EFL word recognition and reading comprehension among 66—11th grade female high school students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Measures included L1 phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and derivational morphological awareness, as well as EFL word recognition and reading comprehension. Background variables including socioeconomic background measured as home-population density and English exposure out of school measured by time spent viewing English television per day were controlled for. Hierarchical regression analyses found L1 phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge and derivational morphological awareness judgment all directly contributed to EFL word recognition. English home language exposure and derivational morphological awareness production directly predicted EFL reading comprehension, and morphological awareness mediated the connection between L1 phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge and EFL reading comprehension. These results highlight the cross-linguistic role of morphological awareness together with home background on EFL reading for female high school students from low socio-economic backgrounds.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abadzi, H. (2006). Efficient learning for the poor: Insights from the frontier of cognitive neuroscience. Washington, DC: The World Bank.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1995). Learning to read in Arabic: Reading, syntactic, orthographic and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Arabic readers. Reading Psychology, 16, 351–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2002). Reading in a root-based-morphology language: The case of Arabic. Journal of Research in Reading, 25, 299–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Said Mansour, M. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic–Hebrew–English-speaking Arab children. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 611–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arafat, S. H., Korat, O., Aram, D., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2016). Continuity in literacy achievements from kindergarten to first grade: A longitudinal study of Arabic-speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9709-x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arar, K., & Oplatka, I. (2016). Current research on Arab female educational leaders’ career and leadership: A review of extant literature and future directions for research. In B. G. Barnett, A. R. Shoho, & A. J. Bowers (Eds.), Challenges and opportunities of educational leadership research and practice: The state of the field and its multiple futures (pp. 87–115). NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonologic and morphologic skills in reading disabled and normal children: Evidence from perception and production of spoken Hebrew. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 876–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141–163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-009-9130-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bouhlila, D. S. (2017). Parents’ education and literacy skills: Evidence on inequality of socioeconomic status in Arab countries. World Development Perspectives, 5, 34–43. Retrieved from https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.wdp.2017.02.006.

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1991). Phonological skills before and after learning to read. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processing in literacy (pp. 37–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., Beeman, M., Hull Davis, L., & Sphraim, G. (1999). Relationship of metalinguistic capabilities and reading achievement for children who are becoming bilingual. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 459–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., & Siegel, L. (2006). A longitudinal study of reading development of Canadian children from diverse linguistic backgrounds. The Elementary School Journal, 107, 135–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C., & Akerman, R. (2006). Social inclusion and reading: An exploration. National Literacy Trust, 1–11. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED496345.pdf.

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 733–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What reading does for the mind. American Educator, 22(1–2), 8–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the second language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dufva, M., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1999). Native language literacy and phonological memory as prerequisites for learning English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 329–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. G., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2000). Socio-economic differences in foundation-level literacy. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 145–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ennemoser, M., & Schneider, W. (2007). Relations of television viewing and reading: Findings from a 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 349–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fender, M. (2008). Arabic literacy development and cross-linguistic effects in subsequent L2 literacy development. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 101–124). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R. L., & Javorsky, J. (1998). Foreign language learning difficulties: An historical perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 248–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R. L., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J., & Bishop-Marbury, A. (1991). Identifying native language difficulties among FL learners in college: A FL learning disability? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 530–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Rueda, R. S., & August, D. (2006). Synthesis: Sociocultural contexts and literacy development. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners (pp. 249–267). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabe, W. (2009). Reading in a second language: Moving from theory to practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hary, B. (1996). The importance of the language continuum in Arabic multiglossia. In A. Elgibali (Ed.), Understanding Arabic: Essays in contemporary Arabic linguistics in honor of El-Said Badawi (pp. 69–90). Cairo: American University in Cairo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes-Hassell, S., & Rodge, P. (2007). The leisure reading habits of urban adolescents. Journal of Adolescents & Adult Literacy, 51(1), 22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., & Goldstein, Z. (2008). English as an additional language word recognition task. Tivon: Oranim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Kuash, S., Ibrahim, R., & Schwartz, M. (2014). How do previously acquired languages affect acquisition of English as a foreign language: The case of Circassian. Written Language and Literacy, 17, 40–61. https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.17.1.03kah.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Schwartz, M., & Share, D. L. (2011). Acquiring the complex English orthography: A tri-literacy advantage? Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 136–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Shimron, J., & Sparks, R. L. (2005). Predicting foreign language reading achievement in elementary school students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 527–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Shimron, J., & Sparks, R. L. (2006). Weak and strong novice readers of English as a foreign language: Effects of first language and socio-economic status. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(1), 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Sparks, R. L., & Goldstein, Z. (2012). English as a Foreign language spelling development: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 343–363. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C. A., White, M. J., & Kremer, K. E. (2005). Developing successful readers: Building early comprehension skills through television viewing and listening. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33(2), 91–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (1997). Orthographic knowledge in L2 lexical processing: A cross-linguistic perspective. In J. Coady & T. Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 35–52). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2008). Impacts of prior literacy experience on second-language learning to read. In K. Koda & A. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 68–96). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K., & Zehler, A. M. (2008). Conceptualizing reading universals, cross linguistic variations, and second language literacy development. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 1–9). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koolstra, C. M., & Beentjes, J. W. J. (1999). Children’s vocabulary acquisition in a foreign language through watching subtitled television programs at home. Educational Technology Research and Development, 47(1), 51–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koolstra, C. M., van der Voort, T. H. A., & van der Kamp, L. J. T. (1997). Television’s impact on children’s reading comprehension and decoding skills: A 3-year panel study. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(2), 128–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition and use: The Taipei Lectures. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (2004). The power of reading. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann and Westport.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2008). Conceptual and methodological issues in comparing metalinguistic awareness across languages. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 39–67). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lam, K., Chen, X., Geva, E., Luo, Y. C., & Li, H. (2012). The role of morphological awareness in reading achievement among young Chinese-speaking English language learners: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1847–1872. https://doi.org/10.1007/s111145-011-9329-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laufer, B. (2003). Vocabulary acquisition in a second language: Do learners really acquire most vocabulary by reading? Some empirical evidence. Canadian Modern Language Review, 59, 567–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levie, R., Ben-Zvi, G., & Ravid, D. (2016). Morpho-lexical development in language impaired and typically developing Hebrew-speaking children from two SES backgrounds. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. https://doi.org/10.1007/s111145-016-9711-3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin-Epstein, N., & Semyonov, M. (1992). Modernization and subordination: Arab women in the Israeli labour-force. European Sociological Review, 8, 39–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. (2017). Working toward a more literate world: Reading intervention commentary. In A. Gove, A. Mora, & P. McCardle (Eds.), Progress toward a literate world: Early reading interventions in low-income countries. New Directions Child and Adolescent Development, 155, 131–141.

  • Ministry of Education, Culture & Sport. (1999). Hamashov haartzi lema’arexet haxinux, Anglit kita xet, [National feedback for educational system, English 8th grade]. Jerusalem: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mokhtari, K., Reichard, C. A., & Gardner, A. (2009). The impact of internet and television use on the reading habits and practices of college students. Journal of Adolescents & Adult Literacy, 52, 609–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbot, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbot, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nassaji, H., & Geva, E. (1999). The contribution of phonological and orthographic processing skills to adult ESL reading: Evidence from native speakers of Farsi. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 241–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, S., & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: An ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(1), 8–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nippold, M. A., Duthie, J. K., & Larsen, J. (2005). Literacy as a leisure activity: Free-time preferences of older children and young adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olshtain, E., Shohamy, E., Kemp, J., & Chatow, R. (1990). Factors predicting success in EFL among culturally different learners. Language Learning, 40, 23–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, E. C. (2004). The relationship between morphological awareness and lexical inference skills for English language learning with Korean first-language back-ground, doctoral dissertation. Discussion Abstracts International, 65 (5), 1761, (UMI No. 3131518).

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Dunlap, S. (2008). Learning to read: General principles and writing system variations. In K. Koda & A. M. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 13–38). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, B., & Lonigan, C. (2005). Social correlates of emergent literacy. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading (pp. 173–187). MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razel, M. (2001). The complex model of television viewing and educational achievement. Journal of Educational Research, 94, 371–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russak, S., & Kahn-Horwitz, J. (2015). English foreign language spelling: Comparisons between good and poor spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 38, 307–330. https://doi.org/10.1111/jrir.12009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Bilingual oral reading fluency and reading comprehension: The case of Arabic/Hebrew (L1)—English (L2) readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 717–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossia and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 559–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., Ibrahim, R., & Kahn-Horwitz, J. (2016). Multi-literate experience as a treasure chest for young learners of English as foreign language. Reading and Writing. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9633-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., & Fowler, A. (2004). Questions people ask about the role of phonological processes in learning to read. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 483–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6, 250–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirin, S. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C., Burns, S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Learning to read with language impairment. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading (pp. 39–412). MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soper, D. (n.d.). Calculator: Post-hoc statistical power for multiple regression. Retrieved from http://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc/calculator.aspx?id=9.

  • Sparks, R. L. (1995). Examining the linguistic coding differences hypothesis to explain individual differences in foreign language learning. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 187–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., & Ganschow, L. (1991). Foreign language learning difficulties: Affective or native language aptitude differences? Modern Language Journal, 75, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., & Ganschow, L. (1993). Searching for the cognitive locus of foreign language learning difficulties: Linking native and foreign language learning. Modern Language Journal, 77, 289–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., Humbach, N., & Javorsky, J. (2006). Native language predictors of foreign language proficiency and foreign language aptitude. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 129–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spolsky, B. (1989). Conditions for second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. NY: The Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suk-Han Ho, C., Pui-Sze Law, T., & Man Ng, P. (2000). The phonological deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 57–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1990). Use of derivational morphology during reading. Cognition, 36, 17–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1986). Linguistic coding and metalinguistic awareness: Their relationship to verbal memory and code acquisition in poor and normal readers. In D. B. Yaden & S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning literacy (pp. 115–141). Postmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Barker, T. A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 243–276). Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Yang, C., & Cheng, C. (2009). The contributions of phonology, orthography, and morphology in Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 291–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeun, S. S. S., Siegel, L. S., & Chen, C. K. K. (2013). Effects of a phonological awareness program on English reading and spelling among Hong Kong Chinese ESL children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 681–704. https://doi.org/10.1007/s111145-012-9383-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahar, R., Cobb, T., & Spada, N. (2001). Acquiring vocabulary through reading: Effects of frequency and contextual richness. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 57, 541–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The data for this study was collected by the second author in the framework of an extended final paper completed in the M.Ed. Foreign Language Program - English Education Specialization, Oranim Academic College of Education.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janina Kahn-Horwitz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kahn-Horwitz, J., Saba, M. Weak English foreign language readers: the cross-linguistic impact of morphological awareness. Read Writ 31, 1843–1868 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9810-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9810-9

Keywords

Navigation